Ñîöèàëüíûé âèä Ëèáåðìàí Ìýòòüþ

179. Dennett, D. C. (1971). Intentional systems. Journal of Philosophy, 68, 87–106.

180. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04), 515–526.

181. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.

182. Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04), 568–570.

183. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128; Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

184. Happ, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.

185. Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337–342; Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14(5), 402–408.

186. Cheney, D. L. (2011). Extent and limits of cooperation in animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10902–10909; Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.

187. Price, B. H., Daffner, K. R., Stowe, R. M., & Mesilam, M. M. (1990). The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. Brain, 113(5), 1383–1393; Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1995). The development of children’s Theory of Mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25–31; Gordon, A. C. L., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a Theory of Mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 70–83.

188. Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition, 93(3), B109–B121.

189. Rottschy, C., et al. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 60, 830–846.

190. Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 316–322; Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., Cho, S. H., Chae, J. H., Lee, S., & Kim, K. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: Stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 29(2), 578–586.

191.  ðàííåì íåéðîïñèõîëîãè÷åñêîì èññëåäîâàíèè íàøëè ñâÿçü ìåæäó ïîâðåæäåíèÿìè ëàòåðàëüíîé ïðåôðîíòàëüíîé êîðû è íåñïîñîáíîñòüþ ðåøàòü çàäà÷è íà ìîäåëü ïñèõè÷åñêîãî. Íî â ðåòðîñïåêòèâå ýòè ñëîæíîñòè ñïèñàëè íà îáùóþ ñëîæíîñòü çàäà÷è. Price, B., Daffner, K., Stowe, R., & Mesulam, M. (1990). The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. Brain, 113, 1383–1393; Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640–656.

192. Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109–128.

193. Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., …, & Eickhoff, S. B. (2011). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based metaanalysis. NeuroImage. 60, 830–846; Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., Padmanabhan, S., Clark, V. P., Karni, A., …, & Neville, H. (1997). Sentence reading: A functional MRI study at 4 Tesla. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 664–686; Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffi ro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 767–773.

194. Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happ, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125(8), 1839–1849.

195. St. Jacques, P. L., Conway, M. A., Lowder, M. W., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Watching my mind unfold versus yours: An fMRI study using a novel camera technology to examine neural differences in self-projection of self versus other perspectives. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1275–1284.

196. Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds). Handbook of Social Psychology, 5th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 143–193; Van Overwalle, F. (2011). A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1589–1599.

197. Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682.

198. Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., …, & Herscovitch, P.(1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2 (15) O PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173–1197; Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475–481.

199. Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (under review). Social by default: Brain activity at rest facilitates social cognition.

200. Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Lpez-Sol, M., Hernndez-Ribas, R., Deus, J., Ortiz, H., …, & Cardoner, N. (2008). Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(28), 9781–9786; Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489–510.

201. Anticevic, A., Repovs, G., Shulman, G. L., & Barch, D. M. (2010). When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2638–2648; Li, C. S. R., Yan, P., Bergquist, K. L., & Sinha, R. (2007). Greater activation of the “default” brain regions predicts stop signal errors. NeuroImage, 38(3), 640–648.

202. James, W. (1950/1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover.

203. Yoshida, W., Seymour, B., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Neural mechanisms of belief inference during cooperative games. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(32), 10744–10751.

204. Coricelli, G., & Nagel, R. (2009). Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(23), 9163–9168.

205. Ïñèõîëîãè, âåðîÿòíî, íå ñîãëàñÿòñÿ, ÷òî ëó÷øèé ñòðàòåãè÷åñêèé îòâåò — ýòî 0. Îáû÷íî ñòðàòåãè÷åñêèå ñïîñîáíîñòè ó÷àñòíèêîâ âàðüèðóþòñÿ, ïîýòîìó îïòèìàëüíûì îòâåòîì áóäåò ÷èñëî áîëüøå íîëÿ.

206. Falk, E. B., Morelli, S. A., Welbourn, B. L., Dambacher, K., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). Creating buzz: The neural correlates of effective message propagation. Psychological Science.

207. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). Automaticity, control, and the social brain. In J. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.). Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind. New York: Guilford; Apperly, I. A., Riggs, K. J., Simpson, A., Chiavarino, C., & Samson, D. (2006). Is belief reasoning automatic? Psychological Science, 17(10), 841–844.

208. Meyer, M. L., Spunt, R. P., Berkman, E. T., Taylor, S. E., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Social working memory: An fMRI study of parametric increases in social cognitive effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1883–1888; Wagner, D. D., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Individual differences in the spontaneous recruitment of brain regions supporting mental state understanding when viewing natural social scenes. Cerebral Cortex, 21(12), 2788–2796.

209. Mckiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394–408.

210. Dumontheil, I., Jensen, S. G., Wood, N. W., Meyer, M. L., Lieberman, M. D., & Blakemore, S. (under review). Influence of dopamine regulating genes on social working memory.

211. Berkman, E., & Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: Theoretical and methodological considerations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 475–493.

212. Griffin, D. W., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference, and human misunderstanding. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 319–359.

213. Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11(1), 32–38.

214. Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338.

Ãëàâà 6. Ñâåò ìîé, «çåðêàëüöå», ñêàæè

215. Rizzolatti, G., Gentilucci, M., Camarda, R. M., Gallese, V., Luppino, G., Matelli, M., & Fogassi, L. (1990). Neurons related to reaching-grasping arm movements in the rostral part of area 6 (area 6a). Experimental Brain Research, 82(2), 337–350.

216. Pellegrino, G. D., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.

217. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.

218. Ramachandran, V. S. (2000). Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evolution. Edge website article: http://­www.edge.org/­3rd_culture/­ramachandran/­ramachandran_p1.html.

219. Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(02), 105–124; Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B. (2009). Is the mirror neuron system involved in imitation? A short review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(7), 975-980; Blakeslee, S. (2006). Cells that read minds. New York Times, January 10, p. 1; Fabrega Jr., H. (2005). Biological evolution of cognition and culture: Off Arbib’s mirror-neuron system stage? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(02), 131–132; Gallese, V. (2001). The shared manifold hypothesis. From mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 33–50.

220. Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2005). Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15(1), 5–26.

221. Ramachandran, V. S. (2000). Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evolution. Edge website article: http://­www.edge.org/­3rd_culture/­ramachandran/­ramachandran_p1.html.

222. Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.

223. Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003). The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(5), 1123–1128.

224. Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H. J., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 42(2), 323–334.

225. Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3), 279–301; Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind reader’s tool kit: Projection and stereotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 340.

226. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.

227. Gallese, V.,Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403; Íà ñàìîì äåëå Ãîðäîí ïðåäâîñõèòèë ýòó èäåþ íà äâàäöàòü ëåò ðàíüøå, íàïèñàâ, ÷òî «èíòåðåñíàÿ âîçìîæíîñòü ïðàêòè÷åñêîé ñèìóëÿöèè — ýòî ïîäãîòîâëåííûé “ìîäóëü”, àâòîìàòè÷åñêè ïîäêëþ÷àåìûé ïðè âîñïðèÿòèè îêðóæàþùèõ»: Gordon, R. M. (2007). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind & Language, 1(2), 158–171.

228. Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 264–274.

229. Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.

230. Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 1229–1243.

231. Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 31(1), 155–166.

232. Lee, H., Simpson, G. V., Logothetis, N. K., & Rainer, G. (2005). Phase locking of single neuron activity to theta oscillations during working memory in monkey extrastriate visual cortex. Neuron, 45(1), 147–156.

233. Heyes, C. (2010). Mesmerising mirror neurons. NeuroImage, 51(2), 789–791.

234. Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527–1531; Catmur, C., Mars, R. B., Rushworth, M. F., & Heyes, C. (2011). Making mirrors: Premotor cortex stimulation enhances mirror and counter-mirror motor facilitation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2352–2362.

235. Newman-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijlen, A. M., & Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience, 10(7), 817–818.

236. Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(27), 9673–9678.

237. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). Automaticity, control, and the social brain. In J. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.). Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind. New York: Guilford.

238. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identifi cation and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94(1), 3.

239. Carver, C. S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 1251.

240. Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1).

241. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Dissociating modality-specific and supramodal neural systems for action understanding. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 3575–3583; Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). An integrative model of the neural systems supporting the comprehension of observed emotional behavior. NeuroImage, 59, 3050–3059; Spunt, R. P., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dissociable neural systems support retrieval of “how” and “why” action knowledge. Psychological Science, 21, 1593-1598; Spunt, R .P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Identifying the what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 63–74; Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: Inferential processes versus action simulation. Current Biology, 17(24), 2117–2121; de Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Current Biology, 18(6), 454–457; Noordzij, M. L., Newman-Norlund, S. E., De Ruiter, J. P., Hagoort, P., Levinson, S. C., & Toni, I. (2009). Brain mechanisms underlying human communication. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 14.

242. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Dissociating modality-specific and supramodal neural systems for action understanding. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 3575–3583.

243. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2013). The busy social brain: Evidence for automaticity and control in the neural systems supporting social cognition and action understanding. Psychological Science, 24, 80–86.

244. James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover, p. 462.

Ãëàâà 7. Ïîäúåìû è ñïàäû

245. Titchener, E. B. (1909). Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of Thought-Processes. New York: Macmillan.

246. The modern use of the term to refer to appreciating another person’s experience can be traced to Husserl’s Ideen and his student Edith Stein’s dissertation published as Stein, E. (1989/1916). On the Problem of Empathy. Washington D.C.: ICS Publications.

247. Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: Progress, pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 675–680.

248. Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nature Neuroscience, 8(7), 955–960.

249. Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11(1), 86–89.

250. Neal, D. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(6), 673–678.

251. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40(3), 655–664; Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(9), 5497–5502.

252. Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). An integrative model of the neural systems supporting the comprehension of observed emotional behavior. NeuroImage, 59, 3050–3059; Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103–134; Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(5661), 1157–1162.

253. Batson, C. D. (1991). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

254. Batson, C. D. (1991). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

255. Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 903–911.

256. Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D., & Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron, 68(1), 149–160.

257. Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). The neural components of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.

258. Cf. Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., Krueger, F., Tura, B., …, & Grafman, J. (2011). Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1735–1742; Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, R., Strenziok, M., …, & Grafman, J. (2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 20084–20089; Inagaki, T. K., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). Neural correlates ofgiving support to a loved one. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74, 3–7.

259. Andy, O. J., & Stephan, H. (1966). Septal nuclei in primate phylogeny: A quantitative investigation. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 126(2), 157–170; Sesack, S. R., Deutch, A. Y., Roth, R. H., & Bunney, B. S. (1989). Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: An anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 290(2), 213–242.

260. Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(6), 419.

261. Heath, R. G. (1972). Pleasure and brain activity in man. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 154(363), 9.

262. Brady, J. V., & Nauta, W. J. (1953). Subcortical mechanisms in emotional behavior: Affective changes following septal forebrain lesions in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 46(5), 339.

263. Carlson, N. R., & Thomas, G. J. (1968). Maternal behavior of mice with limbic lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(3p1), 731; Cruz, M. L., & Beyer, C. (1972). Effects of septal lesions on maternal behavior and lactation in the rabbit. Physiology & Behavior, 9(3), 361–365; Slotnick, B. M., & Nigrosh, B. J. (1975). Maternal behavior of mice with cingulate cortical, amygdala, or septal lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88(1), 118.

264. Inagaki, T. K., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). Neural correlates of giving support to a loved one. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74, 3–7.

265. Insel, T. R., Gelhard, R., & Shapiro, L. E. (1991). The comparative distribution of forebrain receptors for neurohypophyseal peptides in monogamous and polygamous mice. Neuroscience, 43(2), 623–630.

266. Lukas, M., Bredewold, R., Neumann, I. D., & Veenema, A. H. (2010). Maternal separation interferes with developmental changes in brain vasopressin and oxytocin receptor binding in male rats. Neuropharmacology, 58(1), 78–87; Francis, D. D., Champagne, F. C., & Meaney, M. J. (2001). Variations in maternal behaviour are associated with differences in oxytocin receptor levels in the rat. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 12(12), 1145–1148.

267. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

268. Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418; White, S. J., Hill, E. L., Happ, F., & Frith, U. (2009). Revisiting the Strange Stories: Revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Development, 80, 1097–1117.

269. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259; Klin, A. (2003). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 831–846.

270. Frith, U., & Happ, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind.” Cognition, 50(1), 115–132.

271. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613-620.

272. Frith, U., & Happ, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind.” Cognition, 50(1), 115–132.

273. Frith, U., & Happ, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind.” Cognition, 50(1), 115–132; Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (under review). Social by default: Brain activity at rest facilitates social cognition; cf. Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The Empathizing-Systemizing (E-S) Theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 68–80.

274. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have an effect on the ability to develop conversation in children with autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 519–537; Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(4), 415–433.

275. Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(3), 459–474; Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126–129; Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind & Language, 15(1), 123–145.

276. Adrien, J. L., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., Perrot, A., Hameury, L., Larmande, C., & Sauvage, D. (1993). Blind ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon family home movies. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 617–626; Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations of the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 861–876.

277. DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., …, & Kimberlin, C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(3), 264–287.

278. Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299.

279. Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 55(4), 558–562; Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altshuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198; Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30; Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and mirror neuron functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 610–621; Ramachndran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Broken mirrors: A theory of autism. Scientifi c American, 16, 62-69; Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079(1), 15-24.

280. Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altshuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

281. Nieuwenhuis, S., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: A problem of signifi cance. Nature Neuroscience, 14(9), 1105–1107.

282. Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.

283. Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and mirror neuron functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 610–621.

284. Fan, Y. T., Decety, J., Yang, C. Y., Liu, J. L., & Cheng, Y. (2010). Unbroken mirror neurons in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 981–988; Raymaekers, R., Wiersema, J. R., & Roeyers, H. (2009). EEG study of the mirror neuron system in children with high functioning autism. Brain Research, 1304, 113–121.

285. Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Behrmann, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Normal movement selectivity in autism. Neuron, 66(3), 461–469; Marsh, L. E., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2011). Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1511–1519; Martineau, J., Andersson, F., Barthlmy, C., Cottier, J. P., & Destrieux, C. (2010). Atypical activation of the mirror neuron system during perception of hand motion in autism. Brain Research, 1320, 168–175.

286. Southgate, V., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2008). Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 225–229.

287. Bird, G., Leighton, J., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2007). Intact automatic imitation of human and robot actions in autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1628), 3027–3031.

288. Spengler, S., Bird, G., & Brass, M. (2010). Hyperimitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1148–1155.

289. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 21.

290. Markram, H., Rinaldi, T., & Markram, K. (2007). The intense world syndrome: An alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1(1), 77–96; Markram, K., & Markram, H. (2010). The intense world theory: A unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–29.

291. http://­nolongerinabox.wordpress.com/­2012/­09/­19/­on-eyecontact/.

292. Adolphs, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Tranel, D. (2002). Impaired recognition of social emotions following amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1264–1274.

293. Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., & Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron, 39(4), 701.

294. Morris, J. S., hman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdale mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(4), 1680–1685; Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411–418.

295. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(3), 355–364; Critchley, H. D., Daly, E. M., Bullmore, E. T., Williams, S. C., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D. M., …, & Murphy, D. G. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour changes in cerebral blood fl ow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain, 123(11), 2203–2212; Pierce, K., Mller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiformface area in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124(10), 2059–2073.

296. Bachevalier, J. (1991). An animal model for childhood autism: Memory loss and socioemotional disturbances following neonatal damage to the limbic system in monkeys. Advances in Neuropsychiatry and Psychopharmacology, 1, 129–140.

297. Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(3), 137–145.

298. Mosconi, M. W., Cody-Hazlett, H., Poe, M. D., Gerig, G., Gimpel-Smith, R., & Piven, J. (2009). Longitudinal study of amygdala volume and joint attention in 2- to 4-year-old children with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(5), 509; Schumann, C. M., Hamstra, J., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., Lotspeich, L. J., Kwon, H., Buonocore, M. H., …, & Amaral, D. G. (2004). The amygdala is enlarged in children but not adolescents with autism; the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(28), 6392–6401.

299. Witelson, S. F., Kigar, D. L., & Harvey, T. (1999). The exceptional brain of Albert Einstein. Lancet (London, England), 353(9170), 2149–2153.

300. Juranek, J., Filipek, P. A., Berenji, G. R., Modahl, C., Osann, K., & Spence, M. A. (2006). Association between amygdala volume and anxiety level: Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study in autistic children. Journal of Child Neurology, 21(12), 1051–1058.

301. Krysko, K. M., & Rutherford, M. D. (2009). A threat-detection advantage in those with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition, 69(3), 472–480; Kleinhans, N., Johnson, L., Richards, T., Mahurin, R., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., & Aylward, E. (2009). Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(4), 467–475.

302. Munson, J., Dawson, G., Abbott, R., Faja, S., Webb, S. J., Friedman, S. D., …, & Dager, S. R. (2006). Amygdalar volume and behavioral development in autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 686.

303. Samson, F., Mottron, L., Soulires, I., & Zeffi ro, T. A. (2011). Enhanced visual functioning in autism: An ALE meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1553–1581.

304. Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2009). Talent in autism: Hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1377–1383; Blakemore, S. J., Tavassoli, T., Cal, S., Thomas, R. M., Catmur, C., Frith, U., & Haggard, P. (2006). Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 5–13; Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13(3), 215–228; Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Rog, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L., …, & Collet, L. (2004).Increased perception of loudness in autism. Hearing Research, 198(1), 87–92; Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Garver, C. R., Grannemann, B. D., Andrews, A. A., Savla, J. S., …, & Schroeder, J. L. (2006). The pattern of sensory processing abnormalities in autism. Autism, 10(5), 480–494.

305. Neumann, D., Spezio, M. L., Piven, J., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Looking you in the mouth: Abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 194–202.

306. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261; Neumann, D., Spezio, M. L., Piven, J., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Looking you in the mouth: Abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 194–202.

307. Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., …, & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526.

Ãëàâà 8. ×åëîâåê — òðîÿíñêèé êîíü

308. Becher, J. J. (1669). Physica subterranea. Frankfurt.

309. Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167(3914), 86–87.

310. Gallup, G. G. (1977). Self-recognition in primates: A comparative approach to the bidirectional properties of consciousness. American Psychologist, 32(5), 329.

311. Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B., & Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45), 17053–17057; Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(10), 5937–5942.

312. Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

313. Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Identity: Cultural Change and the Struggle for Self. New York: Oxford University Press, p. 153.

314. Kelley W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 785–794.

315. Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 1742–1752.

316. Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds). Handbook of Social Psychology, 5th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 143–193.

317. Tsujimoto, S., Genovesio, A., & Wise, S. P. (2011). Frontal pole cortex: Encoding ends at the end of the endbrain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 169–176; Preuss, T. M., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1991). Myelo- and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 310(4), 429–474.

318. Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114(3), 224–241.

319. Semendeferi, K., Teffer, K., Buxhoeveden, D. P., Park, M. S., Bludau, S., Amunts, K., …, & Buckwalter, J. (2011). Spatial organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. Cerebral Cortex, 21(7), 1485–1497.

320. Hesse, H. (1923). Demian. New York: Boni & Liverright.

321. From Earnshaw’s Infants’ Department in June 1918, as quoted from Smithsonian.com: Jeanne Maglaty, “When Did Girls Start Wearing Pink?” April 8, 2011.

322. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist (C. W. Morris, Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago; Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Scribner.

323. Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, M., Fuligni, A. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: When social perspective-taking informs selfperception. Child Development, 80(4), 1016–1038.

324. Crasilneck, H. B., McCranie, E. J., & Jenkins, M. T. (1956). Special indications for hypnosis as a method of anesthesia. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 162(18), 1606–1608; Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Costantini-Ferrando, M. F., Alpert, N. M., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(8), 1279–1284; Spiegel, H. (1970). A singletreatment method to stop smoking using ancillary self-hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 18(4), 235–250; Surman, O. S., Gottlieb, S. K., Hackett, T. P., & Silverberg, E. L. (1973). Hypnosis in the treatment of warts. Archives of General Psychiatry, 28(3), 439.

325. Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., Mann, T., Harrison, B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Predicting persuasion-induced behavior change from the brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8421–8424.

326. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231.

327. Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Neural activity during health messaging predicts reductions in smoking above and beyond self-report. Health Psychology, 30, 177–185.

328. Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). From neural responses to population behavior: Neural focus group predicts population level media effects. Psychological Science, 23, 439–445.

329. Alain de Botton tweet @alaindebotton 3/5/12, 3:00 a.m.

330. New York Times (1932). Einstein is terse in rule for success. June 20, p. 17.

331. Louis C.K. quote from Interview with Jessica Grose, June 17, 2011, in Slate h2d “Questions for Louis C.K.”

332. Bakan, D. (1971). Adolescence in America: From idea to social fact. Daedalus, 100, 979–995; Fasick, F. A. (1994). On the “invention” of adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14(1), 6–23.

333. Steve Jobs, 2005, Stanford commencement.

Ãëàâà 9. Âñåâèäÿùèé ñàìîêîíòðîëü

334. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.

335. Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-i reactions before age two. Developmental sychobiology, 5(4), 297–305.

336. Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratifi cation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329; Mischel, W., & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(2), 254.

337. Mischel, W., & Moore, B. (1973). Effects of attention to symbolically presented rewards on self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(2), 172.

338. Mischel, W., & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(2), 254.

339. Moore, B., Mischel, W., & Zeiss, A. (1976). Comparative effects of the reward stimulus and its cognitive representation in voluntary delay. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(3), 419.

340. Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratifi cation: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 978.

341. Lehrer, J. (2009). DON’T! The secret of self-control. New Yorker, May 18, pp. 26–32.

342. Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16(12), 939–944.

343. Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., …, & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698; Meier, S., & Sprenger, C. D. (2012). Time discounting predicts creditworthiness. Psychological Science, 23(1), 56–58; Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Bernzweig, J., Karbon, M., Poulin, R., & Hanish, L. (1993). The relations of emotionality and regulation to preschoolers’ social skills and sociometric status. Child Development, 64(5), 1418–1438; Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 978; Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2008). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324; Ct, S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2010). The ability to regulate emotion is associated with greater well-being, income, and socioeconomic status. Emotion, 10(6), 923.

344. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error. New York: Putnam.

345. Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11(3), 249–254; Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252.

346. Shaw, P., Lalonde, F., Lepage, C., Rabin, C., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., …, & Rapoport, J. (2009). Development of cortical asymmetry in typically developing children and its disruption in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(8), 888; Holloway, R. L., & De La Costelareymondie, M. C. (1982). Brain endocast asymmetry in pongids and hominids: Some preliminary findings on the paleontology of cerebral dominance. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 58(1), 101–110; Zilles, K. (2005). Evolution of the human brain and comparative cytoand receptor architecture. In S. Dehaene, J. R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser, & G. Rizzolatti (Eds.). From Monkey Brain to Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books, pp. 41–56.

347. Cohen, J. R., Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Intentional and incidental self-control in ventrolateral PFC. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.). Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 417–440; Cohen, J. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). The common neural basis of exerting self-control in multiple domains. In Y. Trope, R. Hassin, & K. N. Ochsner (Eds.). Self-control. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 141–160.

348. Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170–177.

349. Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116.

350. Casey, B. J., Somerville, L. H., Gotlib, I. H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N. T., Askren, M. K., …, & Shoda, Y. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratifi cation 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(36), 14998–15003.

351. Berkman, E. T., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). In the trenches of real-world self-control: Neural correlates of breaking the link between craving and smoking. Psychological Science, 22, 498–506.

352. Evans, J. S. B., Barston, J. L., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 295–306.

353. Êîãíèòèâíûé ñàìîêîíòðîëü îáû÷íî ñìåùàåòñÿ ê ëåâîìó ïîëóøàðèþ. Íî åñëè îí áëèæå ê õîëèñòè÷åñêîìó (êîãäà ÷åëîâåê ïûòàåòñÿ óñâîèòü ìûñëü èëè óáåæäåíèå), òî, êàê îêàçàëîñü, âîçâðàùàåòñÿ â ïðàâîå ïîëóøàðèå, êàê è äðóãèå îïèñàííûå âèäû ñàìîêîíòðîëÿ.

354. Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition, 87(1), 11–22.

355. Tsujii, T., & Watanabe, S. (2010). Neural correlates of belief-bias reasoning under time pressure: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1320–1326.

356. Tsujii, T., Masuda, S., Akiyama, T., & Watanabe, S. (2010). The role of inferior frontal cortex in belief-bias reasoning: An rTMS study. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 2005; Tsujii, T., Sakatani, K., Masuda, S., Akiyama, T., & Watanabe, S. (2011). Evaluating the roles of the inferior frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule in deductive reasoning: An rTMS study. NeuroImage, 58(2), 640–646.

357. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458.

358. De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684–687.

359. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Kathirgamanathan, U., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Seeing it my way: A case of a selective defi cit in inhibiting self-perspective. Brain, 128(5), 1102–1111; van der Meer, L., Groenewold, N. A., Nolen, W. A., Pijnenborg, M., & Aleman, A. (2011). Inhibit yourself and understand the other: Neural basis of distinct processes underlying Theory of Mind. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2364–2374.

360. Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3), 279–301.

361. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291.

362. Murakami, H. (2008). What I Talk About When I Talk About Running: A Memoir. New York: Knopf, p. vii.

363. Bower, J. E., Low, C. A., Moskowitz, J. T., Sepah, S., & Epel, E. (2007). Benefit finding and physical health: Positive psychological changes and enhanced allostasis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 223–244.

364. Pape, H. C. (2010). Petrified or aroused with fear: The central amygdala takes the lead. Neuron, 67(4), 527–529.

365. Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 48; Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Joural of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410; Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291.

366. Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249.

367. Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577.

368. Lee, T.-W., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2008). Controlling emotional expression: Behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emotional responses. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 104–113.

369. Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229; Phan, K. L., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. J., Uhde, T. W., & Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 210–219; Kalisch, R. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of reappraisal: Time matters. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(8), 1215–1226; Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Oakley, D. A., …, & Dolan, R. J. (2005). Anxiety reduction through detachment: Subjective, physiological, and neural effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), 874–883.

370. Kalisch, R. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of reappraisal: Time matters. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 1215–1226.

371. Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274–281.

372. Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201; Denham, S. A., & Burton, R. (1996). A social-emotional intervention for at-risk 4-year-olds. Journal of School Psychology, 34, 225–245; Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Hanish, L. D., & Spinrad, T. L. (2001). Preschoolers’ spontaneous emotion vocabulary: Relations to likability. Early Education & Development, 12, 11–27; Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Clarke, D. (2002). Emotion regulation in children with specifi c language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 102–111; Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18–23; Mostow, A. J., Izard, C. E., Fine, S., & Trentacosta, C. J. (2002). Modeling emotional, cognitive, and behavioral predictors of peer acceptance. Child Development, 73, 1775–1787.

373. Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science, 331, 211–213.

374. Lieberman, M. D., Inagaki, T. K., Tabibnia, G., & Crockett, M. J. (2011). Subjective responses to emotional stimuli during labeling, reappraisal, and distraction. Emotion, 3, 468–480; Burklund, L. J., Creswell, J. D., Irwin, M. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (under review). The common neural bases of affect labeling and reappraisal.

Ñòðàíèöû: «« 345678910 »»

×èòàòü áåñïëàòíî äðóãèå êíèãè:

«Áðàò áîëîòíîãî êðàÿ» — èñòîðèÿ ïàòðèàðõàëüíîé ñåìüè, æèâóùåé â ÷àùå äðåìó÷åãî ëåñà. Ñëàâÿíñêèé ôîëü...
×òî îáùåãî ó àíàëèòèêà äàííûõ è Øåðëîêà Õîëìñà? Êàê ó Netflix ïîëó÷èëîñü ñîçäàòü 100 %-íûé õèò – ñåð...
Ýòîé ñêàçêå íóæíû èëëþñòðàöèè.Íàðèñóé è ïðèøëè íàì ñâîè ðèñóíêè.Ìîæåò, èìåííî, òû âûèãðàåøü ãëàâíûé ...
Êàêîå äåëî øâåäñêîìó äèïëîìàòó, ïðîøåäøåìó ÷åðåç èñïûòàíèÿ ïåñêàìè è æàðîé Þæíîãî Ñóäàíà, â êîòîðîì ...
 êíèãå ñîäåðæèòñÿ îïèñàíèå ïðèåìîâ ìåòàêîììóíèêàöèè, îòíîñÿùèõñÿ ê òåõíèêàì àêòèâíîãî ñëóøàíèÿ, ïîê...
Êàæäûé ÷åëîâåê æèâåò ñâîþ æèçíü. Íåêîòîðûì, âïðî÷åì, âûïàäàåò ïðîæèòü íåñêîëüêî æèçíåé – êàê àâòîðó ...